majortests.com

Psychology task 1 Essay

Psychology Task 1 Behaviourists explain behaviour as a response to a stimulus and that of the environment that we live in is the stimulus that we respond. They also they say that the past also affects a person’s behaviour. Operant conditioning is learning through punishments and rewards for example positive reinforcement, negative reinforcement, positive punishment and negative punishment. Operant conditioning can also be described as a new behaviour being introduced into the behavioural repertoire of an animal or human. Classical conditioning is when you learn behaviour through association to something for example you might learn to walk more carefully over ice if you have fallen over previously. The social learning approach means that you learn through others for example the media, peers, family, teachers and people in our environment. In classical conditioning, people learn to associate two stimuli when they occur together, such that the response that they originally had by one stimulus is transferred to another. A person learns to produce an existing response to a new stimulus. There was an experiment by Ivan Pavlov who studied dogs. He observed that dog salivated in response to food; they also salivated in response to the footsteps of the attendants which fed the dog. Pavlov decided that he was going to see if he could get the dogs to salivate to the sound of music (metronome). Salivations is an automatic response this means that it is not learnt so this is an unconditioned response (UR), food is a unconditioned stimulus (US), Pavlov then used just a tuning fork and this had no salivating response from the dog, he then decided to pair the tuning fork with food this then produced a unconditioned response Pavlov repeated this a number of times, after conditioning Pavlov then only had to play the tuning fork and the dog salivated this is a conditioned response (CR). In operant conditioning, people learn new behaviours through the penalty of the things they do. If a behaviour they produce is followed by reinforcement then the possibility of that behaviour being repeated increases in the future so the behaviour is strengthened. A consequence can be reinforcing in two ways: either the person gets something good this is called positive reinforcement or they avoid something bad this is called negative reinforcement. On the other hand, if behaviour is followed by a punishment then the likelihood of that behaviour being repeated in future decreases so the behaviour is weakened. Operant conditioning allows people to learn new responses. There is an experiment created by B.F Skinner the experiment was in an operant conditioning chamber also known as a Skinner box, it was used to analyse the behaviour of an animal, the box was used to teach an animal to perform certain actions such as pressing a lever in response to a specific stimuli, for example a light or a sound. When the tester performs the behaviour correctly they are given a reward, if it was not the box delivered a punishment in Skinners experiment this was an electric shock eventually the tester always did the correct behaviour. In the social learning theory there is observational learning the five factors of observational learning is: 1. Availability 2. Attention 3. Retention 4. Reproduction 5. And Motivation For the behaviour to be learnt it need to be performed somewhere an individual can see for example on the TV, on a computer game or observing a peer. Also the person must notice the behaviour, the amount of attention we pay is affected usually by the characteristics of the model you are observing, for example you are more likely to pay attention to an attractive, high-status or is powerful but you are less likely to pay attention to someone with a low status and power. The behaviour that we are observing must be retained you would have to old enough or interested enough to keep the information about this behaviour in their brain. The

Related Documents: Psychology task 1 Essay

psychology task words

Reference: Psychology and Intrinsic Motivation Essay examples

27, 360-371 © 2013 Human Kinetics, Inc. www.TSP-Journal.com APPLIED RESEARCH The Efforts of Sport Psychology Professionals to Assist Sport Administrators in Evaluating Youth Sport Programs Susumu Iwasaki and Mary D. Fry University of Kansas This study highlights how sport psychology professionals can assist sport administrators in evaluating and strengthening youth sport programs. A sport psychology research team provided expertise to two sport administrators to develop a survey to examine their…

Words 7491 - Pages 30

psychology task words

Sport Psychology 1 Essay

Position statements - 1. Definition of sport psychology, 1995 FEPSAC Position Statement # 1 Definition of sport psychology Several studies have revealed the importance and acceptance of sport psychology. However, the crucial question about the real nature of sport psychology is difficult to answer precisely. Although many definitions have been suggested, there is no comprehensive and internationally accepted definition of sport psychology until now. There is, therefore, a need for a clear description…

Words 793 - Pages 4

Does a Positive Gain Spiral of Resources Essay

elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh Does a positive gain spiral of resources, efficacy beliefs and engagement exist?q Susana Llorens a,*, Wilmar Schaufeli b,1, Arnold Bakker b,2, Marisa Salanova a,3 a ´ Department of Psychology, Universitat Jaume I, Campus de Riu Sec s/n 12071, Castellon, Spain b Department of Social and Organizational Psychology, Utrecht University, P.O. Box 80.140, 3508 TC Utrecht, The Netherlands Available online 10 December 2004 Abstract The present study among 110 Spanish…

Words 8308 - Pages 34

Assignment Two Psychology Essay

Assessment Activity Front Sheet Programme: BTEC (Edexcel) Extended Diploma in Health and Social Care Student Name: Group: 1 Task: 2 / 2 Minimum Target Grade: Aspirational Target Grade: Assessor Name: Rachel Ostick I. V. Name: Paula Collins Unit: Unit 8 - Psychological Perspectives for Health and Social Care Assignment Title: Psychological Approaches to Health and Care Practice Date Set: 31/03/14 First Deadline: 02/06/14 Final Deadline: (Learning and review) 23/06/14 Evidence required: Report…

Words 618 - Pages 3

Lexeme and Lexical Decision Task Essay

Research: Lexical Task: recognition time of word frequency. Balota, David A., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and performance. June, 1984 vol 10(3) pp.340-357 Three experiments with 80 undergraduates investigated the impact of 5 lexical variables (instance dominance, category dominance, word frequency, word length in letters, and word length in syllables) on performance in 3 tasks involving word recognition: category verification, lexical decision, and pronunciation. Although…

Words 579 - Pages 3

psychology task words

Essay on Final Report

Context processing related to schizotypy traits Abstract: Background: In order to see whether schizophrenia patients can concentrate on more than one task-cue bi-conditional discrimination task used to exploring context processing in relation with schizotypy. The study conducted by Haddon et al (2011) conveyed that high schizotypy participants showed impaired bi-conditional performances than the low schizotypy participants. Research Aim: The aim of this study is to replicate and generalise the…

Words 1886 - Pages 8

The Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale Essay

The Effects on Psychology The Stanford-Binet Intelligence scale has played a huge role in the field of psychology. Today, there are many different categories of psychology. However, the Stanford-Binet scale fits best in the fields of child psychology, and industrial and organizational psychology (I&O). As explained above this scale is based mainly around the ability to determine gifted children. Many gifted children are used as research subjects in psychology to help aide researchers…

Words 1023 - Pages 5

Essay on performance profile Autosaved

Performance profiling Name:- Kola obalowo Age:- 21 Case history end The client is a 21 year old male student, who has been referred himself to our services as he is seeking to acknowledge if the application of psychology within his sporting activities can enhance his performance; This is his first experience with a psychologist so he remains sceptical about everything, he expects this programme to “somehow influence his physical performance”. The highest level Kola has participated in was…

Words 1167 - Pages 5

Essay Motivational Plan

575 January 12, 2011 There are five basic theories of motivation. 1) Self-determination theory states that people have three basic needs. 2) Attribution theory wants to know why things happen. 3) Expectancy-value theory is interested in a person’s beliefs about abilities. 4) Social cognitive theory states that human behavior comes from their attributes. 5) Goal orientation theory wants to know the reasons for doing tasks. Self-Determination Theory The self-determination theory states that…

Words 1258 - Pages 6

The neuroscience of the brain Essay

Neuroscience is the scientific study of the nervous system.[1] Traditionally, neuroscience has been seen as a branch of biology. However, it is currently an interdisciplinary science that collaborates with other fields such as chemistry, computer science, engineering, linguistics, mathematics, medicine and allied disciplines, philosophy, physics, and psychology. It also exerts influence on other fields, such as neuroeducation[2] and neurolaw. The term neurobiology is usually used interchangeably…

Words 901 - Pages 4

Psychology Tasks Analysis - Research Paper Example

Psychology Tasks Analysis

Extract of sample "Psychology Tasks Analysis"

Thus, the population progresses. Genetic difference drives natural selection, and genetic inheritance conveys adaptive characters from a single generation to the subsequent. If all the associates of a particular species were the same genetically, then genetic variation could not exist and, therefore, no natural selection. The organisms in this species might still diversely reproduce because of their adaptation; however, they would convey the same genes as those that did not manage to reproduce.

Despite advancements in technology and medicine, along with the improved popularity of monogamy, studies indicate that humans carry on developing just like other organisms. Natural and sexual selection still affects humans, however, the factors making some people able to live better, or have better achievements and give birth to more children, have transformed with time and vary in dissimilar populations (Milot et al., 17040).Conferring to Staddon and Cerutti, (140), this is a behaviorist theory that is centered on the main idea that reinforced behaviors are likely to endure while punished behaviors will sooner or later end.

Like all other learning theories, behavioral conditioning theory has both negative and positive aspects. There are numerous weaknesses as well as strengths to this theory. For instance, when children are rewarded for behaving well, they are more likely to go on conducting themselves that way. Without positive motivation or a reward, these children will lack the motivation to continue with good behavior. Additionally, if children misbehave, they should be subjected to some form of negative reinforcement that will build up their behavior.

Subsequently, children who have been punished for acting in a particular manner will be less likely to repeat that behavior. Applying negative and positive reinforcement procedures might change a child’s character problems.

CHECK THESE SAMPLES OF Psychology Tasks Analysis

Work psychology, theory of the meaning of psychology, peoples behavior, declarative knowledge, humanistic approach towards self development, psychology experiment on learning by doing vs learning by viewing, do managers need to develop concrete skills in the political analysis of organizations, psychology ,research methods ,qualitative methodologies.

psychology task words

Psychology task 5000 words

Budget $30-250 CAD

See attached

Need this done in 36 hours max

not a minute over

do not if you think you can't deliver on time

max budget $140

Skills: Education & Tutoring , Online Writing , Research Writing , Technical Writing

Project ID: #7570707

Looking to make some money?

5 freelancers are bidding on average $133 for this job.

Hello! If you want your psychology task to be done by a qualified and professional academic writer; please feel free to contact me at your earliest. I am as excited as you to present this paper in wonderful style. I al More

Hi, I am a professional writer, and I am running a team of native article writers (USA, Uk. Kenya). They are working for me more than two –three years. I do proofreading ( grammarly.com+ premium copyscape ) and sendi More

A proposal has not yet been provided

I have worked for you in past and I delivered best quality. I am ready to work for you again. kindly contact me if you are interested.

Hi there? Am a high quality academic writer specializing in research works. I present quality, 100% original work and meet your time lines. Am interested in your project and would like to work with you. Kindly contact More

Hi, I am interested in doing this project for $100 within 36 hours. Please consider sending details. Thank you.

Welcome, ! Processing …

Link to existing freelancer account.

The email address is already associated with a Freelancer account. Enter your password below to link accounts:

Link your account to a new Freelancer account

Link to your existing Freelancer account

Related Words Logo

Related Words

psychology task words

This tool helps you find words that are related to a specific word or phrase. Also check out ReverseDictionary.org and DescribingWords.io . Here are some words that are associated with ~term~ : . You can get the definitions of these ~term~ related words by clicking on them. Also check out describing words for ~term~ and find more words related to ~term~ using ReverseDictionary.org

Click words for definitions

Our algorithm is scanning multiple databases for related words. Please be patient! :)

Words Related to Another Word

Below is a list of words related to another word . You can click words for definitions. Sorry if there's a few unusual suggestions! The algorithm isn't perfect, but it does a pretty good job for common-ish words. Here's the list of words that are related to another word :

Popular Searches

Words related to ~term~.

As you've probably noticed, words related to " term " are listed above. Hopefully the generated list of term related words above suit your needs.

P.S. There are some problems that I'm aware of, but can't currently fix (because they are out of the scope of this project). The main one is that individual words can have many different senses (meanings), so when you search for a word like mean , the engine doesn't know which definition you're referring to ("bullies are mean " vs. "what do you mean ?", etc.), so consider that your search query for words like term may be a bit ambiguous to the engine in that sense, and the related terms that are returned may reflect this. You might also be wondering: What type of word is ~term~ ?

Also check out ~term~ words on relatedwords.io for another source of associations.

Related Words runs on several different algorithms which compete to get their results higher in the list. One such algorithm uses word embedding to convert words into many dimensional vectors which represent their meanings. The vectors of the words in your query are compared to a huge database of of pre-computed vectors to find similar words. Another algorithm crawls through Concept Net to find words which have some meaningful relationship with your query. These algorithms, and several more, are what allows Related Words to give you... related words - rather than just direct synonyms.

As well as finding words related to other words, you can enter phrases and it should give you related words and phrases, so long as the phrase/sentence you entered isn't too long. You will probably get some weird results every now and then - that's just the nature of the engine in its current state.

Special thanks to the contributors of the open-source code that was used to bring you this list of term themed words: @Planeshifter , @HubSpot , Concept Net , WordNet , and @mongodb .

There is still lots of work to be done to get this to give consistently good results, but I think it's at the stage where it could be useful to people, which is why I released it.

Please note that Related Words uses third party scripts (such as Google Analytics and advertisements) which use cookies. To learn more, see the privacy policy .

Recent Queries

psychology task words

psychology task words

Assignment Help Service

| Plagiarism Free | Secure Payment | Enjoy Excellent Grades|

Are you struggling with your classes or need urgent help with your busy schedule?

Hire Someone To Do Your Assignment

Why boost grades at classaider.

psychology task words

Provide assignment help experts with instructions

Our assignment-help team follow the instructions provided and combine their many years of experience to produce correct answers. Navigate to our  essay ordering form  and fill out the details of your essay. These include the type of essay, subject, instructions, number of sources, referencing style, and deadline. Make sure to upload any relevant instruction files of reading materials.

First-time customers will provide an email address, phone number, and password. These details are used to create an account securely.

Pay someone to write your assignment.

Pay securely using a bank card or PayPal.  Our site is equipped with advanced PCI DSS Le vel 1 payment processors and is certified by leading cardholders, including Visa, Mastercard, American Express, and Discover. Your payment information is always secure, and we never have access. You can rest assured that all site transactions are safe and secure. Once paid, one of our fast essay typers will start working on your assignment.

psychology task words

Get straight A’s from essay writers

Once our best essay typers complete your paper, you will receive an email stating your order is completed. Our goal is always to deliver 100% satisfaction, but if you feel that your order was not accurately followed, request a revision. Our quality assurance team will work to address any issues.

Once satisfied, approve your order and leave an honest review about our services. Your feedback will be posted anonymously on our testimonials page. See  what others have said about our services .

Classaider provides reliable and affordable assignment help USA services

Cheap assignment help.

Get the best affordable assignment help service in the market. You will set the best price.

Fast Assignment Help

We are here for your urgent essay or task . We also respect your demands and deliver on time.

Get Excellent Grades

Your project will be assigned to an expert US, UK, or CA writer within your subject area for fast delivery.

Is university assignment help really safe? This is probably one of the questions in your mind right now. The answer is Yes and No for many free homework help websites, but at classaider.com, we say Yes. This is a 100% secure homework help website in that your information is protected, can never be leaked, and our services are custom.

All papers are written from scratch and are by no means resold to another student. We understand that cheating is wrong, and we cannot take you that route. So demand maximum privacy while ordering from classaider.com or other homework-helping websites.

Despite tight deadlines and regardless of the difficulty of your task, classaider.com always guarantees a pass. Excellence is of the essence, and students deserve value for their money. Our excellence comes from many years of tutoring and hiring highly qualified graduate or postgraduate writers. All our writers have agreed to the following principles.

Assignment Help Service

Order Cheap Assignment Writing Help

Sometimes the life of a student becomes overwhelming, and happiness fades. You have a lot of work to complete within a short deadline or do not understand what the professor expects. That is why you need a trusted essay writer, and at Classaider.com, we offer the most student-oriented academic writing service 24/7.

Our experts are ready for your projects, from college essays to affordable university research papers. Do not worry about tricky schedules and challenging assignments. Hire someone to write your essay.

Order assignment help now from our class aiders, and get it done quickly! We want students to enjoy their learning by offering assistance where they get stuck. Our team of essay writers will deliver whatever service you ask for. Do you need a research summary or an outline to complete your essay?

What is the best assignment help website?

Classaider.com helps students succeed in college and university  by offering the most reliable homework help. If you are looking for someone who understands your homework, then you are in the right place. Our tutors are the top homework helper on the internet.  See their ratings here.

Order from the most trusted online assignment to boost grades or help with a challenging cla ss . The fact that our writing service is available 24/7 makes us reliable for students from around the world. If you are looking for someone you can trust with your school life, classaider.com is the answer. We are proof of reliable essay assignment help service.

Please do not hesitate to ask for assistance; we have writers for a wide range of subjects ready to work on your essay for Geography, International Law, Sociology, Philosophy, Culture, Ethics, and other subjects.

classaisder assignment help

What makes the best assignment help service?

At classaider.com, our team of assignment helpers comprises professional essay writers with both advanced education and extensive experience. They hail from prestigious schools across North America and other parts of the globe, with some featuring their scholarly work in academic publications. When you entrust your work to us, you can rest assured that you will receive unparalleled support from writers devoted to your academic excellence.

Our team wants to see you pass and appreciate the value of your money. Buy an essay today and enjoy excellent grades. We provide 100% original papers written by professional tutors hired through a rigorous screening system. We assist in academic papers of various complexity, and other personalized services, as well as research materials for aid purposes only, are among our offerings. All of the materials on our website should be referenced properly.

essay writers

What Our Clients Are Saying

psychology task words

Thank you for checking in. it was a pleasure working with you. The assignment was completed quickly with high-quality results. Classaider.com is the best site for students. Thanks again.

JANET MORRIS

psychology task words

As an entertainer and blogger, I always turn to Classaider.com when I create blog posts on various topics. I just share my ideas in a list and let their writer come up with excellent writing. They are an example of a company that works hard to deliver.

WILLIE BROWN

psychology task words

I asked my writer on ClassAider.com to help me compose a personal essay about my charity work at the local church. It is not easy for me to express myself in words, so I needed some privacy and a good person who would appreciate my thoughts and ideas.

SEAN FISHER

Hire An Assignment Helper for

Assignment help, essay help, homework, research papers, term papers, this, course work, discussion posts, and more

Frequently asked questions

Classaider.com is a professional essay writing service that provides high-quality academic writing assistance to students and professionals. Our team of expert writers can handle a wide range of academic writing tasks, including essays, research papers, dissertations, and more.

We offer a wide range of academic writing services, including essay writing, research paper writing, case study analysis, thesis writing, and more. No matter what type of assignment you need help with, we have a team of experts who can handle it.

Placing an order on classaider.com is simple and straightforward. All you have to do is provide us with the details of your assignment, including the type of paper, the length, and any specific requirements you have. You can then choose a writer and make a payment through our secure payment system.

You can choose a writer for your order by browsing through the profiles of our writers. Each writer has a profile that includes information about their qualifications, experience, and ratings from previous customers. You can also read samples of their work to get an idea of their writing style.

You can communicate with your writer through our messaging system. This allows you to exchange messages and files with your writer, and receive updates on the progress of your order.

Yes, your personal information is kept confidential. We take the privacy and security of our customers very seriously and use advanced encryption technology to protect your information.

Once your paper is completed, it will be sent to you via email. You can then download the paper and review it. If you have any revisions or feedback, you can send it back to your writer for revision.

If you are not satisfied with your paper, you can request revisions or request a refund. Our goal is to provide you with a high-quality paper that meets your expectations, and we are committed to making sure you are satisfied with your order.

psychology task words

Provide details on what you need help with along with a budget and time limit. Questions are posted anonymously and can be made 100% private.

psychology task words

Studypool matches you to the best tutor to help you with your question. Our tutors are highly qualified and vetted.

psychology task words

Your matched tutor provides personalized help according to your question details. Payment is made only after you have completed your 1-on-1 session and are satisfied with your session.

PSYC 1101 ATC Psychology Understanding What Psychologist Do Essay

User Generated

ubzrjbexpbafhgyvat

Description

 Understanding What Psychologist Do

One of the best resources on the web is the American Psychological Association’s website which can be found at:

http://www.apa.org

This extensive website has a plethora of information on psychology from the largest professional organization in psychology. Your textbook author discusses 15 areas of study within the field of psychology. This will often pique the interest of the beginning student and often leads to the question, “What career opportunities are there in psychology?” The APA website can help answer this question. Access the website at the above address. Once there, click on the “careers” link. Then you will be able to click on the several different links including brochures. You could go directly to one of the brochures by accessing:

http://www.apa.org/careers/resources/guides/careers.aspx

Once you access this site you will see career options listed and short explanations of each. Please select one a career option that interest you and explain why using this internet resource and any material provided.

psychology task words

Explanation & Answer

psychology task words

Attached. Running head: CAREER FIELD IN PSYCHOLOGY Career Field in Psychology Details Name Institutional Affiliation Date 1 CAREER FIELD IN PSYCHOLOGY 2 Clinical Psychology As a career path, clinical psychology is most interesting to me. Based on the concepts and the facts which it has, it helps in solving a number of psychological problems in the medical setting (Kramer, Bernstein & Phares, 2019). Clinical psychology refers to a field of psychological study in the medical setting that employs interaction with the patients and the healthcare workers. It involves understanding of vital psychological concepts and focusing on the needed strategies and policies for maintaining peace in the medical setting. In the clinical setting, clinical psychology is essential since it helps in the counselling of the various people in the healthcare setting regarding the most important concepts. Some of the basic strategies involve controlling violence and shaping the thinking of the professionals in the setting. I love clinical psychology because it promotes peace in the medical setting and also promotes quality interaction and a good mood effectively (Kramer, Bernstein & Phares, 2019). Clinical psychology is good in the ...

psychology task words

24/7 Homework Help

Stuck on a homework question? Our verified tutors can answer all questions, from basic  math  to advanced rocket science !

psychology task words

Similar Content

Related tags.

methodology Argosy University development development psychology assignment lucifer effect therapies allopathic medicines environment anxiety education academic

Flowers For Algernon

by Alice Walker

Harry Potter and the Sorcerers Stone

by J. K. Rowling

The Glass Castle

by Jeannette Walls

The English Patient

by Michael Ondaatje

The 48 Laws of Power

by Robert Greene

by Elie Wiesel

The Life-Changing Magic of Tidying Up

by Marie Kondo

Death on the Nile

by Agatha Christie

Sharp Objects

by Gillian Flynn

psychology task words

working on a homework question?

Studypool, Inc., Tutoring, Mountain View, CA

Studypool is powered by Microtutoring TM

Copyright © 2023. Studypool Inc.

Studypool is not sponsored or endorsed by any college or university.

Ongoing Conversations

psychology task words

Access over 20 million homework documents through the notebank

psychology task words

Get on-demand Q&A homework help from verified tutors

psychology task words

Read 1000s of rich book guides covering popular titles

psychology task words

Sign up with Google

psychology task words

Sign up with Facebook

Already have an account? Login

Login with Google

Login with Facebook

Don't have an account? Sign Up

Proficient Writers

complete 150 word psychology task atc

Stuck with a difficult assignment no time to get your paper done feeling confused if you’re looking for reliable and timely help for assignments, you’ve come to the right place. we promise 100% original, plagiarism-free papers custom-written for you. yes, we write every assignment from scratch and it’s solely custom-made for you..

Writerbay.net

We’ve proficient writers who can handle both short and long papers, be they academic or non-academic papers, on topics ranging from soup to nuts (both literally and as the saying goes, if you know what we mean). We know how much you care about your grades and academic success. That's why we ensure the highest quality for your assignment. We're ready to help you even in the most critical situation. We're the perfect solution for all your writing needs. Get a 15% discount on your order using the following coupon code SAVE15

psychology task words

Contact Us: +1 (940) 305-2632

Send us a WhatsApp message

Edxecel GCSE Psychology 9-1 Social Influence key word task sheet

Edxecel GCSE Psychology 9-1 Social Influence key word task sheet

Subject: Psychology

Age range: 14-16

Resource type: Worksheet/Activity

ninachadwick72

Last updated

4 September 2018

pdf, 87.79 KB

This worksheet is given at the beginning of the topic and students complete the descriptions as a starter for each lesson.

Tes paid licence How can I reuse this?

Your rating is required to reflect your happiness.

It's good to leave some feedback.

Something went wrong, please try again later.

This resource hasn't been reviewed yet

To ensure quality for our reviews, only customers who have purchased this resource can review it

Report this resource to let us know if it violates our terms and conditions. Our customer service team will review your report and will be in touch.

Not quite what you were looking for? Search by keyword to find the right resource:

People also looked at

Original research article, exploring the neural representation of novel words learned through enactment in a word recognition task.

psychology task words

Vocabulary learning in a second language is enhanced if learners enrich the learning experience with self-performed iconic gestures. This learning strategy is called enactment. Here we explore how enacted words are functionally represented in the brain and which brain regions contribute to enhance retention. After an enactment training lasting 4 days, participants performed a word recognition task in the functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) scanner. Data analysis suggests the participation of different and partially intertwined networks that are engaged in higher cognitive processes, i.e., enhanced attention and word recognition. Also, an experience-related network seems to map word representation. Besides core language regions, this latter network includes sensory and motor cortices, the basal ganglia, and the cerebellum. On the basis of its complexity and the involvement of the motor system, this sensorimotor network might explain superior retention for enactment.

Introduction

In foreign language instruction, novel vocabulary is mainly taught by means of audio-visual strategies such as listening and comprehension activities ( Graham et al., 2014 ). At home, learners are confronted with bilingual lists that decay fast ( Yamamoto, 2014 ). Memory research has demonstrated that self-performed gestures accompanying words and phrases during learning enhance vocabulary retention compared to reading and/or listening ( Zimmer, 2001 ). The effect of gestures on verbal memory, enactment effect ( Engelkamp, 1980 ; Engelkamp and Krumnacker, 1980 ; Engelkamp and Zimmer, 1984 ) or subject performed task effect ( Cohen, 1981 ), has proven to be robust. In the 1980s and 1990s, enactment of words and phrases was successfully tested in various populations, including children, young, and elderly people ( Bäckman and Nilsson, 1985 ), subjects with cognitive and mental impairments ( Mimura et al., 1998 ), and Alzheimer patients ( Karlsson et al., 1989 ) by means of recognition and free and cued recall tests. In recent years, an increasing number of behavioral studies have documented the positive effect of enactment also in second language word learning in both the short and in the long term (for a review, see Macedonia, 2014 ). More recently, the combined value of enactment and physical exercise has been investigated in elementary bilingual instruction ( Mavilidi et al., 2015 ; Toumpaniari et al., 2015 ).

Over the decades, different theories have accounted for the enactment effect. Nearly 40 years ago, these theories based their evidence on behavioral experiments and were nourished by observation and a large portion of intuition. With the advent of neuroscience, these theories have gained additional empirical ground. They have been partially validated through combined experiments employing tools from both disciplines, i.e., behavioral psychology and brain imaging as we describe in the next sections.

The first theory asserts that a gesture performed by learners during the acquisition of a novel word leaves a motor trace in memory ( Engelkamp and Krumnacker, 1980 ; Engelkamp and Zimmer, 1984 , 1985 ; Nyberg et al., 2001 ). In neuroscience, a trace is an experience-related component in the functional network representing the word ( Pulvermüller, 2002 ); neuroimaging studies have proven the existence of the trace. During acoustic and audio-visual recognition of words learned through enactment, motor cortices become active ( Nyberg et al., 2001 ; Masumoto et al., 2006 ; Eschen et al., 2007 ; Macedonia et al., 2011 ; Mayer et al., 2015 ). More generally, this theory can be embedded in the framework of embodied cognition ( Barsalou, 2008 ), where a concept entails sensory, motor and/or emotive components pertaining to the corresponding sensory, motor and/or affective systems engaged during experience ( Jirak et al., 2010 ). The trace can be detected by means of brain imaging that locates brain activity during word recognition/retrieval. This activity is due to simulation processes that reactivate neural ensembles originally involved and interconnected during the experience ( Dijkstra and Post, 2015 ).

Another theory attributes the enactment effect to mental imagery . Saltz and Donnenwerthnolan (1981) suggested that performing a gesture to a word triggers the mental image associated with the word. Thereafter the double representation (verbal and visual) enhances the word's retention. This view is connected to Paivio's dual-coding theory ( Paivio, 1969 , 1971 ), which maintains that an image paired to a word has an impact on the word's retention because the processing of both word and image engages different channels and exploits both potentials. Imagery related to L2 word learning has also been tested in a brain imaging experiment: Macedonia et al. (2011) had subjects learn L2 words with two sets of gestures, iconic and semantically unrelated. Audiovisual presentation of words learned with semantically unrelated gestures elicited activity in a network engaged in cognitive control. These results were interpreted as detection of the mismatch between the mental image a person has of a word and the gesture used during training. In other studies, mismatches between L1 words and gestures were seen as activating a network denoting incongruence ( Holle and Gunter, 2007 ; Holle et al., 2008 ). Altogether, in recent decades a growing body of evidence has demonstrated that language and gesture represent two aspects of the same communicative system ( Goldin-Meadow, 1999 ; Bernardis and Gentilucci, 2006 ; Kelly et al., 2010 ) and that they share neural substrates (for a review, see Andric and Small, 2012 ).

In a few behavioral studies, the enactment effect has also been explained in terms of complexity of word representation ( Knopf, 1992 ; Kormi-Nouri, 1995 ; Macedonia, 2003 ; Macedonia and Knösche, 2011 ). In these studies, the term complexity was used in a descriptive way: The authors asserted that a written word enriched by a gesture is represented in memory in a more complex way because additional perceptive modalities are engaged. Neuroscience has demonstrated that the brain codes, represents and stores information connected to a word on the basis of the sensory input provided ( Pulvermüller, 2001 , 2005 ; Pulvermüller and Fadiga, 2010 ). Hence gestures make the word's representation richer and more complex. In traditional L2 instruction, learning a written word or listening to it leads to a representation that is bare of multiple sensorimotor experiences as in L1. Gestures as a learning strategy add sensorimotor and proprioceptive information to the word in L2. Thereby the word's representation becomes more complex and elaborative.

A further explanation for the enactment effect focuses on enhanced attention . Supporters of this theory assert that learners performing a gesture connected to a word's semantics are more attentive than those who only read verbal information or hear the word ( Backman et al., 1993 ; Knudsen, 2007 ; Muzzio et al., 2009a , b ; Pereira et al., 2012 ). Attention is a basic component of retention as it induces representational stability in the hippocampus ( Muzzio et al., 2009a ; Aly and Turk-Browne, 2016 ).

The superior retrievability of L2 words learned through enactment has also been accounted for in terms of depth of encoding ( Quinn-Allen, 1995 ; Macedonia, 2003 ; Tellier, 2008 ; Kelly et al., 2009 ; Macedonia et al., 2011 ; Krönke et al., 2013 ). The concept of deep and shallow encoding goes back to the “Level of Processing Framework” (LOP) by Craik and Tulving (1975) . In LOP, sensory processing—hearing a word—is shallow information encoding that leads to poor memory performance ( Craik and Lockhart, 1972 ). By contrast, semantic processing by selecting semantic features of a word in a task is deep and leads to durable memorization ( Hunt and Worthen, 2006 ). Furthermore, deep processing is achieved by integrating novel information with pre-existing knowledge and creating distinctiveness during encoding. Thus, deep processing could be accomplished through gestures. Performing a gesture means selecting arbitrary features related to a word that represent its semantics. Furthermore, gestures can integrate novel information, i.e., the word in L2, with pre-existing semantic knowledge about the word in L1. Although depth of processing has been taken as an explanation for successful memorization in many studies on enactment, the brain mechanisms associated with depth are not fully understood. In an early review article, Nyberg (2002) connected deep processing with brain activity in frontal and medial temporal brain regions. More recently, Galli (2014) reviews functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies on tasks with shallow vs. deep encoding; the author concludes that brain regions engaged in shallow encoding represent a subset of those involved in deep encoding. Galli concludes that shallow and deep encoding might have varying network topographies depending on the kind of stimulus processed and the specificity of the encoding tasks.

While early enactment research attributed the effect to only one of the above reasons, recent research indicates that the different accounts mirror different aspects of enacting verbal information. A gesture accompanying a novel word creates a motor trace, triggers a mental image, encodes more deeply, and obviously engages more attention than only reading the word. All these processes are performed in networks interacting in brain topography and time.

Another theory on enactment, the system-oriented approach pursued by Engelkamp (2001) , has not received much attention in the scientific discourse. Following on the line of memory subsystems ( Engelkamp and Zimmer, 1994 ), Engelkamp advances the hypothesis that enactment, because of the “physical properties of the ongoing stimulus” might engage “more than one memory system.” More explicitly, encoding a written word as such (a character string) involves explicit , i.e., declarative memory. However, encoding a gesture, an action , stores the information related to the word in procedural memory. Accordingly, because enactment of verbal information engages “systems that are obligatorily activated dependently on the stimulus modality” combines both declarative and procedural memory during learning. Engelkamp observes that “there are systems that are not automatically activated given a specific stimulus, but that can be strategically activated, for instance, when a specific task is given.” In other words, the procedural system is not automatically engaged when we learn words by reading them or listening to them. However, the procedural system can be strategically involved by accompanying the words with gestures. Gestures therefore lead to memory enhancement because—by the nature of the stimulus—they engage procedural memory in word storage. Daprati et al. (2005) also address the possible involvement of procedural memory for words encoded by enactment. The study is conducted on healthy individuals and on schizophrenic patients. The latter show deficits in awareness states but procedural memory is intact ( Danion et al., 2001 ). Healthy subjects take advantage of the enactment effect but patients suffering from schizophrenia do not. This result does not confirm the engagement of procedural memory in learning through enactment; however, schizophrenia has been reported to be a motor awareness disturbance ( Frith et al., 2000 ) with deficits in monitoring of self-generated actions ( Frith, 1987 ). This might explain the failure of enactment in this specific population.

Despite the progress in enactment research, we still lack a complete picture of how novel words learned with iconic gestures are functionally mapped in the brain. The present study aims to show the different experience-related components of the word network. To this end, we use a section of a fMRI dataset originally acquired to investigate differences in retention between words learned with iconic gestures and words learned with semantically unrelated gestures ( Macedonia et al., 2011 ). We designed that study in order to test word retention learned with iconic and semantically unrelated gestures. At that time, it was not clear which component was crucial to retention: the motor component or iconicity. We found that words accompanied by iconic gestures were better retained than words learned with semantically unrelated gestures and that this correlated with stronger activity in the motor cortices. Furthermore, words learned with semantically unrelated gestures elicited a network for cognitive control, possibly denoting a mismatch between an internal image of the word and the gesture presented while learning.

In the present study, we extract from the dataset those events that are related to the recognition of words learned with iconic gestures. By analyzing the BOLD response elicited during recognition of the L2 words, we seek to localize networks involved in learning. We hypothesize that these networks include the sensorimotor modalities engaged during the process. Further, we follow Engelkamp's system-oriented approach and pose the question of whether other brain structures related to procedural memory—besides motor cortices, as demonstrated in previous studies—may be involved in a word's representation. If so, this could provide evidence for the engagement of procedural memory in word learning through enactment.

Participants, Behavioral Training Procedure, and Results

Eighteen participants (mean age 23.44, M = 25, SD = 1.38, 10 females, 8 males) memorized 92 words of Vimmi, an artificial corpus created in order to avoid associations to languages known to the subjects and conforming to Italian phonotactic rules ( Macedonia et al., 2011 ). During the training, lasting for 4 days, 2 h daily, the novel words were accompanied either by 46 iconic ( McNeill, 1992 ) or by 46 semantically unrelated gestures such as stretching one's arms. Participants watched videos of an actress performing the gestures and enunciating the novel words (Figure 1 ). Simultaneously, the written word in Vimmi and in German appeared on a screen. Thereafter, participants were cued to repeat the word in Vimmi and to perform the corresponding gesture. Participants were randomly divided in two subgroups that trained both sets of words and gestures in a counterbalanced way. The 92 words were subdivided in blocks of 6 + 6 + 6 + 5 items. Within each block, every vocabulary item was randomized and presented daily 13 times. From the second until the fifth experiment day, memory performance was assessed through cued translation tests from German into Vimmi and vice versa. Starting from day 2, before the training participants were given a randomized list of the 92 trained words to be translated into the other language (duration 7.5 min for every list). Additionally the same test was administered after ~60 days.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Screen shot of an iconic gesture from the video used during training . It represents the Vimmi word bae (Engl. pepper mill). During the training, participants were cued to perform the gesture as they said the word after reading and hearing it.

We performed a repeated measures ANOVA with the factors training and time (iconic and semantically unrelated gestures) and time (DAY 01, DAY 02, DAY 03, DAY 04). Significantly better results in word retention were achieved with iconic gestures in both the short and long term. For both translation directions, memory performance was significantly better for words learned with iconic gestures, i.e., German into Vimmi = Training F (1, 32) = 22.86, p < 0.001 and Vimmi into German = F (1, 32) = 15.20, p < 0.001. Additionally, around 60 days after training, memory performance was tested again by means of a paired free recall test. Participants were first given an empty sheet and were asked to write down as many words as they could remember in either one or the other language with the corresponding translation. This test mirrors the capacity of a second language learner to retrieve the word in both languages. The free recall test showed superior memory results for words learned with iconic gestures F (1, 28) = 122.18, p < 0.001 (Figure 2 ).

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Training results for the cued translation test from German into Vimmi and vice versa (merged data) and results for the paired free recall test (day 60) . Words encoded through iconic gestures are significantly better retrieved at all time points. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.

fMRI Experiment

After the behavioral learning phase, fMRI data were acquired during an audio-visual word recognition task. In the scanner, participants were audio-visually presented 92 words that they had previously trained and 23 novel words. Further 23 silent events represented the baseline. A single item was presented at each trial beginning with a fixation cross for 300 ms followed by the Vimmi word for 1 s. The interstimulus interval was 8 s. Participants read the words on a back-projection screen mounted behind their heads in the bore of the magnet. Audio files with an approximate duration of 1000 ms were played when the word was shown. Participants were instructed to press a key with their left hand if the word was unknown. Altogether the scanning comprised 138 trials with the trained and the novel words and the silent events. All items were balanced across the presentation. The whole experimental session lasted 23 min (Figure 3 ).

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Scanning procedure .

Neuroimaging Parameters

Functional scanning was performed with the following imaging parameters: BOLD sensitive gradient EPI sequence, TR = 2000 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°, acquisition bandwidth = 100 Hz. We acquired 20 axial slices (4 mm thick, 1 mm interslice distance, FOV 19.2 cm, data matrix of 64 × 64 voxels, inplane resolution of 3 × 3 mm) every 2000 ms on a 3-T Bruker (Ettlingen, Germany) Medspec 30/100 system. Prior to functional data acquisition, we obtained a T1-weighted modified driven equilibrium Fourier transform (MDEFT) image (data matrix 256 × 256, TR = 130 ms, TE = 10 ms) with a non-slice-selective inversion pulse that was followed by a single excitation of each slice ( Norris, 2000 ). This anatomical image (which was acquired in the same orientation as the functional images) was co-registered with a previously obtained high-resolution whole-head 3-D brain image: 128 sagittal slices, 1.5-mm thickness, FOV 25.0 × 25.0 × 19.2 cm, data matrix of 256 × 156 voxels. Thereafter, the same registration parameters were applied to the functional images. The fMRI experiment was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Leipzig (Germany).

Data Analysis

For the present paper, we considered only the data related to the words learned through iconic gestures and contrasted them with the baseline silence. We intended to explore the topography of experience-related word networks. We analyzed the functional data with the Lipsia software package ( Lohmann et al., 2001 ). Functional data were corrected for motion and for the temporal offset between the slices. Thereafter, we aligned the functional slices with a 3D stereotactic coordinate reference system. We acquired the registration parameters on the basis of the MDEFT slices, thereby achieving an optimal match between the slices and the individual 3D reference dataset, standardized to the Talairach stereotactic space ( Talairach and Tournoux, 1988 ). We transformed the functional slices using trilinear interpolation, so that the resulting functional slices were aligned with the stereotactic coordinate system according to the registration parameters. During pre-processing, we further smoothed the data with a Gaussian filter of 10 mm FWHM, and a temporal high-pass filter with a cut-off frequency of 1/100 Hz. The data were entered as statistics using general linear regression with pre-whitening ( Worsley et al., 2002 ). By using the Yule–Walker equations from the least squares residuals, we estimated autocorrelation parameters. They were also used to whiten both data and design matrices. Thereafter, we re-estimated the linear model using least squares on the whitened data to produce estimates of effects and their standard errors. We generated the design matrix using the canonical hemodynamic response function ( Friston et al., 1998 ). Subsequently, we generated contrast images by computing the difference between the parameter estimates of the iconic gestures condition and the baseline, i.e., silence. We entered all contrast images into a second-level Bayesian analysis. Compared with null hypothesis significance, this analysis has a high reliability in small-group statistics with high within-subject variability ( Friston et al., 2008 ). For display reasons, Bayesian probabilities (1- p ) were finally transformed to z -values.

fMRI Results

Our data were acquired during a word recognition task when participants lying in the scanner were visually and acoustically presented words that they had previously learned with iconic gestures, and unknown words, in random order. The whole brain analysis of the contrast between all words learned with iconic gestures vs. the baseline silence revealed haemodynamic responses in a number of regions as listed in Table 1 . As fMRI lacks temporal resolution for neural processes, our visualization of regional oxygenated blood flow seems to reflect processes of perception, attention and word selection that occurred during scanning, as well as the functional neural representation of the words (Figure 4 ).

www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Results of fMRI main contrast (Silence—Iconic Gestures) .

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. fMRI Results . Main contrast for words learned with iconic gestures vs. silence. Learning through iconic gestures creates extended sensorimotor networks that resonate upon audio-visual word presentation. The networks map the modalities engaged during learning. The color-coded areas show clusters with high Bayesian posterior probability of condition. The bar represents the z -values.

Audio-Visual Perception and Attention

During task execution in the scanner, sensorial perception and word recognition occur. We found related bilateral neural activity in the thalamus, which is known to process incoming information and to relay it to related specific areas in the cortex ( Mitchell et al., 2014 ). Further, auditory cortices BA 38 along with BA 41 and BA 42 (the transverse temporal areas) responded upon acoustic word perception. In addition to processing auditory information, these areas also access stored representations of words ( Scott and Wise, 2004 ). Other regions were engaged in the task: BA 22 maps sound and word meaning ( Zhuang et al., 2014 ), the left fusiform gyrus BA 37 is implicated in reading ( McCandliss et al., 2003 ), and the supramarginal gyrus BA 40 mediates word recognition ( Stoeckel et al., 2009 ; Wilson et al., 2011 ). Also the frontal eye field BA 8 was active and possibly engaged in attention processes ( Esterman et al., 2015 ).

Lexical Recognition

Zhuang et al. (2014) describe lexical recognition as consisting of two related processes: competition and selection. In competition, cohort candidates, i.e., words sharing some phonological feature(s) with the word presented compete with each other for a match. These cohort words are stored in memory areas that become active upon search. We found activity in an extended memory network engaged in word competition, including the right hippocampus ( Smith et al., 2011 ; Huijgen and Samson, 2015 ), the para-hippocampal gyrus, BA 27, BA 36 bilaterally ( Squire and Dede, 2015 ), and the left temporal lobe BA 38 ( St. Jacques et al., 2011 ). In our data, the process of selection involved the left inferior frontal gyrus and also its right counterpart, BA 44, 46, the left BA 47, cortical regions found previously during the completion of this task ( Heim et al., 2005 ; Rodd et al., 2012 ; Zhuang et al., 2014 ), and the insula, which is also engaged in word processing ( Zaccarella and Friederici, 2015 ). Contrary to this last study, we did not find involvement of the pars triangularis of the inferior frontal gyrus BA 45. Instead, we detected activity in BA 9, a region known to be active in working memory mediating selection tasks, in interplay with BA10, and in the anterior cingulate cortex BA 32 and BA 34 ( Zhang et al., 2003 ), areas also present in our network.

Cognitive Control

Our results suggest that mechanisms of cognitive control might also contribute to the network engaged in word recognition. We found increased activity in the anterior cingulate cortex BA 31 and 34 as well as in its posterior portion BA 31, in the middle and superior frontal gyri BA 9, and in the frontopolar prefrontal cortex BA 10. Previous studies bundled these regions into a network that detects input diverging from a stored template and suppressing it ( Botvinick et al., 2001 ; Cole and Schneider, 2007 ). Hence cognitive control could account for adequate item selection, suppression of cohort candidates, and novel words presented during the task. However, our analysis does not allow discernment of whether the cingulate cortices and BA 10 contribute to selection within the network for cognitive control or whether selection processes and dedicated networks include the network of cognitive control.

Experience-Related Sensorimotor Word Networks

Our investigation aimed to detect how words learned with iconic gestures are functionally mapped into neural tissue. In addition to the core language network ( Friederici, 2011 ) described in the preceding section we found a number of premotor, motor, and sensorimotor areas that were activated during word recognition. Most remarkably, upon audio-visual presentation of the words (note that no videos of the actress performing gestures were shown during the scanning procedure), the brain images unveiled activity in large portions of the left premotor cortex BA 6, which is engaged in movement preparation and simulation. The right counterpart was minimally involved. We attribute this imbalance to the fact that all subjects were right-handed ( Tettamanti et al., 2005 ). Activity in the premotor cortices as preparation of words referring to motor acts is well documented in numerous neuroimaging studies in which words were presented either visually or acoustically or both ( Hauk et al., 2004 ; Pulvermuller, 2005 ; D'Ausilio et al., 2009 ; Cappa and Pulvermüller, 2012 ; Berent et al., 2015 ). In our study, the pattern of response to audio-visual word presentation also involved the left primary motor cortex, where BA 4 and BA 7 play a role in motor sequence coordination, visuo-motor coordination, planning of complex movements, and proprioception ( Baker et al., 2012 ). Additionally, our data revealed involvement of the basal ganglia, i.e., left putamen, left and right caudate, left substantia nigra, and the cerebellum, which bilaterally contribute to motor emulation processes ( Lotze and Halsband, 2006 ; Ridderinkhof and Brass, 2015 ). We ascribed the engagement of the motor regions during word recognition to the experience collected by our subjects while learning. In retrieval, motor acts are unconsciously simulated as associated to the phoneme and letter sequence(s). Similarly, primary somatosensory cortices BA 3 and BA 5, also present in our analysis, converge to create a proprioceptive trace within the network related to gesture execution ( Pleger and Villringer, 2013 ). Perception of space and body location mediated by the supramarginal gyrus BA 40 and the angular gyrus BA 39 are also part of a word's representation ( Blanke, 2012 ). Besides involvement of sensorimotor areas, activity in BA 18, a visual association area, reflects the complex image processing that occurs during training and the reactivation of a mental image ( Lambert et al., 2004 ). Subjects were cued to read the words and watch the videos of an actress performing the iconic gestures. Involvement of the right fusiform gyrus might thus mirror the input, i.e., the actress' face ( Morris et al., 2007 ) and body ( Soria Bauser and Suchan, 2015 ) might also be mapped into the word's representation. Altogether, these data reflect the sensorimotor input processed during learning. The contrast images provide evidence of word learning as a cognitive process grounded in embodied experiences ( Pulvermuller, 1999 ).

The present study is explorative. Also, this fMRI-data analysis has a limitation: it cannot disentangle the different processes that occurred in the scanner, i.e., word perception, recognition, and the functional neural mapping of the words created through sensorimotor experiences. Brain areas engaged in multiple functions partially overlap in the different processes, i.e., sensorial processing, the recognition task and sensorimotor emulation of the words.

However, the data provide insight into the functional representation of words in a foreign language learned through iconic gestures. The results support Engelkamp's and Krummnacker's seminal theory on enactment (1980), which proposed that performing a gesture when memorizing a word leaves a motor trace in the word's representation. Furthermore, our results can be embedded in theories of embodied language that have emerged in the past decade. Based on neuroscientific evidence, these studies maintain that language (along with other processes in cognition) is grounded in bodily experiences created with our sensorimotor systems ( Barsalou, 1999 , 2008 ; Pulvermuller, 1999 ; Gallese and Lakoff, 2005 ; Fischer and Zwaan, 2008 ; Jirak et al., 2010 ; Glenberg and Gallese, 2012 ). When seeing a fruit basket, children get to grasp a fruit. Holding the fruit, children smell it, put it in their mouth, taste it, drop it, pick it up, squeeze it, and feel the pulp and dripping juice; while pointing to the fruit while caregivers produce a sequence of sounds [ˈɒr.ɪndʒ], orange , the “concept's name” ( Oldfield and Wingfield, 1965 ). Connecting all sensorimotor experiences with the sound, children try to reproduce it. By doing so, children combine multiple bodily experiences related to the fruit and create an embodied concept connected to a sound string that becomes a part of this representation. One day children will learn to write the sound sequence. At that point, children acquire another system—graphemes—in order to label the concept and to communicate about these experiences in a written way. Spoken and written words thus become a component of the concept, and as such they become connected with the embodied concept itself. Considering this, words are not abstract units of the mind ( Fodor, 1976 , 1987 ). Instead, words are grounded to a great amount in bodily experiences ( Gallese and Lakoff, 2005 ). A number of studies conducted over the past decade have demonstrated the involvement of the body in conceptual representations. Most of these studies have used written words that participants read silently in the fMRI scanner. Reading action words activated motor cortices ( Kemmerer et al., 2008 ; Cappa and Pulvermüller, 2012 ). This process occurred selectively depending on the effector of the body involved in the action ( Carota et al., 2012 ). Action words (such as kick, pick , and lick ) that refer to actions performed with leg, arm or mouth elicited activity in regions of the motor cortex controlling their movement ( Hauk et al., 2004 ). González et al. (2006) made participants read odor words and found brain activity in regions that are not related only to the task, i.e., canonical language areas involved in reading, but more interestingly in olfactory brain regions. Note that participants had no perception of odor. Similarly, mere reading of gustatory words such as salt engaged gustatory regions in the brain ( Barrós-Loscertales et al., 2012 ).

In recent years, embodiment research has shown that a good portion of abstract words are also grounded in bodily experiences. In the fMRI scanner, Moseley et al. (2012) had participants read abstract emotion words such as fear, dread , and spite . In the subjects' brains, the researchers detected activity in emotional networks, but more interestingly in those portions of the premotor cortex engaged in movement preparation for arm- and face-related gestures. Such gestures ground the social expression of feelings. Anecdotally speaking, if asked to demonstrate the concept of grief, in Western culture we might produce a certain facial expression or mime wiping our eyes to indicate crying. Hence even if, linguistically, the word is abstract, our interpretation of it is embodied. We have learned the spoken or written word for a concept that is not abstract but related to emotional and bodily experiences.

Additionally, metaphors involving body parts also are reported to evoke embodied reactions in the brain. Boulenger et al. (2009) asked whether somatotopic responses in the motor cortex occur during reading of metaphoric sentences such as “John grasped the idea” compared to literal sentences such as “John grasped the object.” The results showed activity in motor cortices for both metaphoric and literal sentences. Another study (with magnetic encephalography) produced similar results ( Boulenger et al., 2012 ). Lacey et al. (2012) had participants read sentences such as “She has steel nerves.” and “Life is a bumpy road.” that contained texture metaphors, along with control sentences such as “She is very calm.” and “Life is a challenging road.” Texture metaphors induced activity in the somatosensory cortex and more specifically in texture-selective areas. Altogether, the reviewed studies suggest that interaction with the world creates brain topographies of concepts and reflects a word's semantics ( Pulvermüller, 2002 ; Moseley and Pulvermüller, 2014 ).

Our data do not overtly support the hypothesis on enactment that attributes memory enhancement to visual imagery ( Backman et al., 1993 ; Knudsen, 2007 ; Muzzio et al., 2009a , b ). However, it stands to reason that the word network also comprises a mental image of the gesture's execution from a first-person perspective; hence the preparation to perform the gesture with the premotor cortex, supplementary motor area ( Park et al., 2015 ), motor cortices, basal ganglia, and cerebellum are involved. However, the word network might also comprise a kinetic mental image from a second-person perspective, i.e., the subject's unconscious rehearsal of the actress during gesture performance. This perspective could be mapped in the higher visual association areas, the fusiform gyrus, the supramarginal, and the angular gyrus as described in the results section of this paper. Other regions, e.g., hippocampus, posterior cingulate cortex, medial prefrontal cortex, and angular gyrus, identified in imagery also are contained in the activated network and found in other studies ( Huijbers et al., 2011 ; Ridderinkhof and Brass, 2015 ).

Network Complexity and Memory

Our results confirm one of the theories on enactment asserting that complexity of word representation determines memory enhancement ( Knopf, 1992 ; Kormi-Nouri, 1995 ; Macedonia, 2003 ). Theoretically, this position is embedded in connectionist models of memory. In connectionism, a concept can be described by means of networks representing and storing information ( McClelland, 1985 ; McClelland and Rumelhart, 1985 ; McClelland and Rogers, 2003 ). Concept networks consist of nodes and edges. Nodes can be seen not only on an abstract level but also in a more biological understanding as neural assemblies and cortical areas wiring together on different scales upon synchronous stimulus processing ( Douglas and Martin, 2004 ; Singer, 2013 ). During learning, synchronous firing of neurons ( coincident activation ) leads to changes in the weight of connections ( Hebb, 1949 ) among neurons. Through these changes in connections, neurons (re)organize in functional units in different dimensions that process and store information. According to connectionist models, concepts are not stored locally in the brain. Instead, concepts are represented in a distributed way ( Lashley, 1950 ). Thus, a concept is not considered as a single unit but as a pattern clustering different components wired together during learning. A growing body of evidence has demonstrated this position in neuroscientific studies (for a review, see Pulvermüller, 2013 ). In connectionist networks, word retrieval is driven by spreading activation within the network. Activation starts at one or more nodes, depending on the input, and triggers activity in the whole network that ends after the search has been completed by inhibition ( McClelland, 1985 ). The classic example described by McClelland et al. (1986) is the rose. The smell and the appearance of the flower are experienced synchronously. After a certain number of experiences, the two components become interconnected. Visual processing of a rose will activate the network. Hence the node storing the smell will be reached by activity and the smell sensation will be triggered even in absence of a real rose. Twenty years later, González et al. fMRI-study ( González et al., 2006 ) connected neuroscientific evidence to McClelland's rose example.

One basic assumption in neural networks theories is that complexity in representation makes memories stable and longer lasting ( Klimesch, 1994 ). The more nodes a concept has, the more stable the concept's representation is. In fact, if a node within the network decays, activity in the network can be started from other nodes and information can be restored. For example, a word in L2 that has been learned with a picture will be more complex in its representation than a word that has been learned in its written form. This will enhance the word's memorability, as shown in a recent study by Takashima et al. (2014) . Paradoxically, if a word network in L2 consists of only one node, for example a string of sounds in a foreign language, decay could affect it fatally. A gesture accompanying a word engages many brain regions and therefore provides a complex representation of the word. It enriches the sound or character string with sensorimotor information, it makes its representation complex, and it enhances retrievability compared to audio-visual learning only. This is the case in both the short ( Macedonia and Knösche, 2011 ) and the long term ( Macedonia and Klimesch, 2014 ) when the network is impacted by decay.

Procedural Memory for Words Learned with Iconic Gestures

Our data also allow a further interpretation. Considering the high involvement of the motor system in the word recognition task, as described in the Results Section, it becomes plausible to assume that procedural memory might be engaged in word learning. Procedural memory is implicit, long-term, and grounded in the motor system when a person acquires a skill. In cognitive science, vocabulary theoretically is situated in the domain of declarative memory ( Tulving and Madigan, 1970 ; Ullman, 2004 ; Cabeza and Moscovitch, 2013 ; Squire and Dede, 2015 ). However, because of the procedure used to acquire the words (iconic gestures accompanying the words, i.e., well defined motor acts/programs), it stands to reason that both declarative and procedural memory systems might interact and jointly accomplish word storage and retrieval. In activation Table 1 , we find brain regions typically mediating declarative memory ( Nikolin et al., 2015 ), i.e., hippocampus, para-hippocampal ( Nadel and Hardt, 2011 ), and the fusiform gyri ( Ofen et al., 2007 ), as well as regions within the prefrontal cortex, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ( Blumenfeld and Ranganath, 2007 ), and the medial temporal lobe ( Mayes et al., 2007 ). This provides evidence that words learned during our experiment are stored in declarative memory. At the same time, our activation list reports brain regions mediating procedural memory, in addition to (pre)motor regions, the basal ganglia ( Barnes et al., 2005 ; Yin and Knowlton, 2006 ; Wilkinson and Jahanshahi, 2007 ) and the cerebellum ( D'Angelo, 2014 ). Thus, word learning, if accompanied by gestures seems to recruit both memory systems. This might be responsible for superior memory performance in storage and retrieval of verbal information. Our results are in line with literature considering declarative and procedural memory as interacting as opposed to being distinct ( Davis and Gaskell, 2009 ), and with a more recent review of patient and animal studies indicating that the medial temporal lobe and basal ganglia mediate declarative and procedural learning, respectively, depending on task demands ( Wilkinson and Jahanshahi, 2015 ).

In this paper, we were interested in the functional neural representation of novel words learned with iconic gestures. Our study is explorative and one of our aims was to detect brain regions that play a special role in memory enhancement. Besides networks engaged in attention and word recognition, we found a word network that maps the words according to the modalities engaged in the learning process. We attribute the superior memory results induced by gestures to the complexity of the network. Complex sensorimotor networks for words store verbal information in an extended way, conferring stability on the word's representation. Our brain data also shows that learning words with gestures possibly engages both declarative and procedural memory. The involvement of both memory systems thus explains why learning is enhanced and information decay is delayed as shown in behavioral long-term studies. The implications for education are clear: gestures should be used in second language lessons in order to enhance vocabulary learning.

Author Contributions

MM Experimental design, stimuli creation and recording, data acquisition and analysis, paper writing. KM Supervision in data acquisition and analysis.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by the Cogito Foundation, Wollerau, Switzerland by a grant to MM.

Aly, M., and Turk-Browne, N. B. (2016). Attention stabilizes representations in the human hippocampus. Cereb. Cortex 26, 783–796. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andric, M., and Small, S. L. (2012). Gesture's neural language. Front. Psychol. 3:99. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00099

Bäckman, L., and Nilsson, L. G. (1985). Prerequisites for lack of age differences in memory performance. Exp. Aging Res. 11, 67–73. doi: 10.1080/03610738508259282

Backman, L., Nilsson, L. G., and Nouri, R. K. (1993). Attentional demands and recall of verbal and color information in action events. Scand. J. Psychol. 34, 246–254. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.1993.tb01119.x

Baker, K. S., Piriyapunyaporn, T., and Cunnington, R. (2012). Neural activity in readiness for incidental and explicitly timed actions. Neuropsychologia 50, 715–722. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.12.026

Barnes, T. D., Kubota, Y., Hu, D., Jin, D. Z., and Graybiel, A. M. (2005). Activity of striatal neurons reflects dynamic encoding and recoding of procedural memories. Nature 437, 1158–1161. doi: 10.1038/nature04053

Barrós-Loscertales, A., González, J., Pulvermüller, F., Ventura-Campos, N., Bustamante, J. C., Costumero, V., et al. (2012). Reading salt activates gustatory brain regions: fMRI evidence for semantic grounding in a novel sensory modality. Cereb. Cortex 22, 2554–2563. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr324

Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behav. Brain Sci. 22, 577–609. discussion: 610–560. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x99002149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59, 617–645. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639

Berent, I., Brem, A. K., Zhao, X., Seligson, E., Pan, H., Epstein, J., et al. (2015). Role of the motor system in language knowledge. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 1983–1988. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1416851112

Bernardis, P., and Gentilucci, M. (2006). Speech and gesture share the same communication system. Neuropsychologia 44, 178–190. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.05.007

Blanke, O. (2012). Multisensory brain mechanisms of bodily self-consciousness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13 , 556–571. doi: 10.1038/nrn3292

Blumenfeld, R. S., and Ranganath, C. (2007). Prefrontal Cortex and long-term memory encoding: an integrative review of findings from neuropsychology and neuroimaging. Neuroscientist 13, 280–291. doi: 10.1177/1073858407299290

Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol. Rev. 108, 624–652. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.624

Boulenger, V., Hauk, O., and Pulvermüller, F. (2009). Grasping ideas with the motor system: semantic somatotopy in idiom comprehension. Cereb. Cortex 19, 1905–1914. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn217

Boulenger, V., Shtyrov, Y., and Pulvermüller, F. (2012). When do you grasp the idea? MEG evidence for instantaneous idiom understanding. Neuroimage 59, 3502–3513. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.011

Cabeza, R., and Moscovitch, M. (2013). Memory systems, processing modes, and components: functional neuroimaging evidence. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 8 , 49–55. doi: 10.1177/1745691612469033

Cappa, S. F., and Pulvermüller, F. (2012). Cortex special issue: language and the motor system. Cortex 48, 785–787. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.04.010

Carota, F., Moseley, R., and Pulvermüller, F. (2012). Body-part-specific representations of semantic noun categories. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 1492–1509. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00219

Cohen, R. L. (1981). On the generality of some memory laws. Scand. J. Psychol. 22, 267–281. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.1981.tb00402.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cole, M. W., and Schneider, W. (2007). The cognitive control network: integrated cortical regions with dissociable functions. Neuroimage 37, 343–360. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.071

Craik, F. I., and Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. J. Exp. Psychol. 104, 268–294. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.104.3.268

Craik, F. I. M., and Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing - framework for memory research. J. Verbal Learning Verbal Behav. 11, 671–684. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(72)80001-X

D'Angelo, E. (2014). The organization of plasticity in the cerebellar cortex: from synapses to control Prog . Brain Res. 210, 31–58. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-63356-9.00002-9. Available online at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/bookseries/00796123

CrossRef Full Text

Danion, J. M., Meulemans, T., Kauffmann-Muller, F., and Vermaat, H. (2001). Intact implicit learning in schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 158, 944–948. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.6.944

Daprati, E., Nico, D., Saimpont, A., Franck, N., and Sirigu, A. (2005). Memory and action: an experimental study on normal subjects and schizophrenic patients. Neuropsychologia 43, 281–293. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.11.014

D'Ausilio, A., Pulvermüller, F., Salmas, P., Bufalari, I., Begliomini, C., and Fadiga, L. (2009). The motor somatotopy of speech perception. Curr. Biol. 19, 381–385. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.01.017

Davis, M. H., and Gaskell, M. G. (2009). A complementary systems account of word learning: neural and behavioural evidence. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 364, 3773–3800. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0111

Dijkstra, K., and Post, L. (2015). Mechanisms of embodiment. Front. Psychol. 6:1525. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01525

Douglas, R. J., and Martin, K. A. C. (2004). Neuronal circuits of the neocortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 419–451. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144152

Engelkamp, J. (1980). Some studies on the internal structure of propositions. Psychol. Res. 41, 355–371. doi: 10.1007/BF00308880

Engelkamp, J. (2001). “Action memory - a system-oriented approach,” in Memory for Action - A Distinct Form of Episodic Memory? eds H. Zimmer, R. L. Cohen, M. J. Guynn, J. Engelkamp, R. Kormi-Nouri, and M. A. Foley (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 49–96.

PubMed Abstract

Engelkamp, J., and Krumnacker, H. (1980). Imaginale und motorische Prozesse beim Behalten verbalen Materials. Z. Exp. Angewandte Psychol. 27, 511–533.

Engelkamp, J., and Zimmer, H. D. (1984). Motor program information as a separable memory unit. Psychol. Res. 46, 283–299. doi: 10.1007/BF00308889

Engelkamp, J., and Zimmer, H. D. (1985). Motor programs and their relation to semantic memory. German J. Psychol. 9, 239–254.

Google Scholar

Engelkamp, J., and Zimmer, H. D. (1994). Human Memory: A Multimodal Approach . Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber.

Eschen, A., Freeman, J., Dietrich, T., Martin, M., Ellis, J., Martin, E., et al. (2007). Motor brain regions are involved in the encoding of delayed intentions: a fMRI study. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 64, 259–268. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.09.005

Esterman, M., Liu, G., Okabe, H., Reagan, A., Thai, M., and Degutis, J. (2015). Frontal eye field involvement in sustaining visual attention: evidence from transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neuroimage 111, 542–548. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.01.044

Fischer, M. H., and Zwaan, R. A. (2008). Embodied language: a review of the role of the motor system in language comprehension. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 61, 825–850. doi: 10.1080/17470210701623605

Fodor, J. A. (1976). The Language of Thought. Hassocks: Harvester Press.

Fodor, J. A. (1987). Psychosemantics: The Problem of Meaning in the Philosophy of Mind . Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Friederici, A. D. (2011). The brain basis of language processing: from structure to function. Physiol. Rev. 91, 1357–1392. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00006.2011

Friston, K., Chu, C., Mourão-Miranda, J., Hulme, O., Rees, G., Penny, W., et al. (2008). Bayesian decoding of brain images. Neuroimage 39, 181–205. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.08.013

Friston, K. J., Fletcher, P., Josephs, O., Holmes, A., Rugg, M. D., and Turner, R. (1998). Event-related fMRI: characterizing differential responses. Neuroimage 7, 30–40. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0306

Frith, C. D. (1987). The positive and negative symptoms of schizophrenia reflect impairments in the perception and initiation of action. Psychol. Med . 17, 631–648. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700025873

Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S. J., and Wolpert, D. M. (2000). Explaining the symptoms of schizophrenia: abnormalities in the awareness of action. Brain Res. Rev. 31 , 357–363. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(99)00052-1

Gallese, V., and Lakoff, G. (2005). The Brain's concepts: the role of the Sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 22, 455–479. doi: 10.1080/02643290442000310

Galli, G. (2014). What makes deeply encoded items memorable? Insights into the levels of processing framework from neuroimaging and neuromodulation. Front. Psychiatry 5:61. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00061

Glenberg, A. M., and Gallese, V. (2012). Action-based language: a theory of language acquisition, comprehension, and production. Cortex 48, 905–922. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.04.010

Goldin-Meadow, S. (1999). The role of gesture in communication and thinking. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 419–429. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01397-2

González, J., Barros-Loscertales, A., Pulvermüller, F., Meseguer, V., Sanjuán, A., Belloch, V., et al. (2006). Reading cinnamon activates olfactory brain regions. Neuroimage 32, 906–912. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.03.037

Graham, S., Santos, D., and Francis-Brophy, E. (2014). Teacher beliefs about listening in a foreign language. Teach. Teach. Educ. 40, 44–60. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2014.01.007

Hauk, O., Johnsrude, I., and Pulvermüller, F. (2004). Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron 41, 301–307. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00838-9

Hebb, D. O. (1949). The Organization of Behavior. A Neuropsychological Theory. New York, NY; London: John Wiley & Sons; Chapman & Hall.

Heim, S., Alter, K., Ischebeck, A. K., Amunts, K., Eickhoff, S. B., Mohlberg, H., et al. (2005). The role of the left Brodmann's areas 44 and 45 in reading words and pseudowords. Cogn. Brain Res. 25, 982–993. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.022

Holle, H., and Gunter, T. C. (2007). The role of iconic gestures in speech disambiguation: ERP evidence. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 1175–1192. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.7.1175

Holle, H., Gunter, T. C., Rüschemeyer, S.-A., Hennenlotter, A., and Iacoboni, M. (2008). Neural correlates of the processing of co-speech gestures. Neuroimage 39, 2010–2024. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.055

Huijbers, W., Pennartz, C. M. A., Rubin, D. C., and Daselaar, S. M. (2011). Imagery and retrieval of auditory and visual information: neural correlates of successful and unsuccessful performance. Neuropsychologia 49, 1730–1740. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.051

Huijgen, J., and Samson, S. (2015). The hippocampus: a central node in a large-scale brain network for memory. Rev. Neurol. (Paris). 171, 204–216. doi: 10.1016/j.neurol.2015.01.557

Hunt, R. R., and Worthen, J. B. (2006). Distinctiveness and Memory. New York, NY; Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Jirak, D., Menz, M. M., Buccino, G., Borghi, A. M., and Binkofski, F. (2010). Grasping language – a short story on embodiment. Conscious. Cogn. 19, 711–720. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.06.020

Karlsson, T., Backman, L., Herlitz, A., Nilsson, L. G., Winblad, B., and Osterlind, P. O. (1989). Memory improvement at different stages of Alzheimers-Disease. Neuropsychologia 27, 737–742. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(89)90119-X

Kelly, S. D., McDevitt, T., and Esch, M. (2009). Brief training with co-speech gesture lends a hand to word learning in a foreign language. Lang. Cogn. Process. 24, 313–334. doi: 10.1080/01690960802365567

Kelly, S. D., Ozyürek, A., and Maris, E. (2010). Two sides of the same coin: speech and gesture mutually interact to enhance comprehension. Psychol. Sci. 21, 260–267. doi: 10.1177/0956797609357327

Kemmerer, D., Castillo, J. G., Talavage, T., Patterson, S., and Wiley, C. (2008). Neuroanatomical distribution of five semantic components of verbs: evidence from fMRI. Brain Lang. 107, 16–43. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.09.003

Klimesch, W. (1994). The Structure of Long-Term Memory: A Connectivity Model of Semantic Processing . Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Knopf, M. (1992). Gedächtnis für Handlungen. Funktionsweise und Entwicklung . Ph.D. thesis, University of Heidelberg, Heidelberg.

Knudsen, E. I. (2007). Fundamental components of attention. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 30, 57–78. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094256

Kormi-Nouri, R. (1995). The nature of memory for action events: an episodic integration view. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 7, 337–363. doi: 10.1080/09541449508403103

Krönke, K.-M., Mueller, K., Friederici, A. D., and Obrig, H. (2013). Learning by doing? The effect of gestures on implicit retrieval of newly acquired words. Cortex 49, 2553–2568. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.11.016

Lacey, S., Stilla, R., and Sathian, K. (2012). Metaphorically feeling: comprehending textural metaphors activates somatosensory cortex. Brain Lang. 120, 416–421. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.12.016

Lambert, S., Sampaio, E., Mauss, Y., and Scheiber, C. (2004). Blindness and brain plasticity: contribution of mental imagery?: an fMRI study. Cogn. Brain Res. 20, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.12.012

Lashley, K. S. (1950). “In search of the engram,” in Society of Experimental Biology Symposium No. 4; Physiological Mechanisms in Animal Behaviour , ed R. J. Pumphrey (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 545–482.

Lohmann, G., Muller, K., Bosch, V., Mentzel, H., Hessler, S., Chen, L., et al. (2001). LIPSIA–a new software system for the evaluation of functional magnetic resonance images of the human brain. Comput. Med. Imaging Graph. 25 , 449–457. doi: 10.1016/S0895-6111(01)00008-8

Lotze, M., and Halsband, U. (2006). Motor imagery. J. Physiol. Paris 99, 386–395. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2006.03.012

Macedonia, M. (2003). Sensorimotor Enhancing of Verbal Memory Through “Voice Movement Icons” During Encoding of Foreign Language (German: Voice Movement Icons. Sensomotorische Encodierungsstrategie zur Steigerung der Quantitativen und Qualitativen Lerneffizienz bei Fremdsprachen). Ph.D. thesis, University of Salzburg.

Macedonia, M. (2014). Bringing back the body into the mind: gestures enhance word learning in foreign language. Front. Psychol. 5:1467. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01467

Macedonia, M., and Klimesch, W. (2014). Long-term effects of gestures on memory for foreign language words trained in the classroom. Mind Brain Educ. 8, 74–88. doi: 10.1111/mbe.12047

Macedonia, M., and Knösche, T. R. (2011). Body in mind: how gestures empower foreign language learning. Mind Brain Educ. 5, 196–211. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-228X.2011.01129.x

Macedonia, M., Müller, K., and Friederici, A. D. (2011). The impact of iconic gestures on foreign language word learning and its neural substrate. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 982–998. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21084

Masumoto, K., Yamaguchi, M., Sutani, K., Tsuneto, S., Fujita, A., and Tonoike, M. (2006). Reactivation of physical motor information in the memory of action events. Brain Res. 1101, 102–109. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.05.033

Mavilidi, M.-F., Okely, A. D., Chandler, P., Cliff, D. P., and Paas, F. (2015). Effects of integrated physical exercises and gestures on preschool children's foreign language vocabulary learning. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 27, 413–426. doi: 10.1007/s10648-015-9337-z

Mayer, K. M., Yildiz, I. B., MacEdonia, M., and Von Kriegstein, K. (2015). Visual and motor cortices differentially support the translation of foreign language words. Curr. Biol. 25, 530–535. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.11.068

Mayes, A., Montaldi, D., and Migo, E. (2007). Associative memory and the medial temporal lobes. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 126–135. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.12.003

McCandliss, B. D., Cohen, L., and Dehaene, S. (2003). The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 293–299. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00134-7

McClelland, J. L. (1985). Distributed models of cognitive processes. Applications to learning and memory. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 444, 1–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1985.tb37576.x

McClelland, J. L., and Rogers, T. T. (2003). The parallel distributed processing approach to semantic cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 310–322. doi: 10.1038/nrn1076

McClelland, J. L., and Rumelhart, D. E. (1985). Distributed memory and the representation of general and specific information. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 114, 159–197. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.114.2.159

McClelland, J. L., Rumelhart, D. E., and Hinton, G. E. (1986). “The appeal of parallel distributed processing,” in Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition , eds D. E. Rumelhart, J. L. McClelland, and P. R. Group (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 3–44.

McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal About Thought . Chicago, IL; London: University of Chicago Press.

Mimura, M., Komatsu, S., Kato, M., Yashimasu, H., Wakamatsu, N., and Kashima, H. (1998). Memory for subject performed tasks in patients with Korsakoff syndrome. Cortex 34, 297–303. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70757-3

Mitchell, A. S., Sherman, S. M., Sommer, M. A., Mair, R. G., Vertes, R. P., and Chudasama, Y. (2014). Advances in understanding mechanisms of thalamic relays in cognition and behavior. J. Neurosci. 34, 15340–15346. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3289-14.2014

Morris, J. P., Pelphrey, K. A., and McCarthy, G. (2007). Face processing without awareness in the right fusiform gyrus. Neuropsychologia 45, 3087–3091. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.020

Moseley, R., Carota, F., Hauk, O., Mohr, B., and Pulvermüller, F. (2012). A role for the motor system in binding abstract emotional meaning. Cereb. Cortex 22, 1634–1647. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr238

Moseley, R. L., and Pulvermüller, F. (2014). Nouns, verbs, objects, actions, and abstractions: local fMRI activity indexes semantics, not lexical categories. Brain Lang. 132, 28–42. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.03.001

Muzzio, I. A., Kentros, C., and Kandel, E. (2009a). What is remembered? Role of attention on the encoding and retrieval of hippocampal representations. J. Physiol. 587, 2837–2854. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.172445

Muzzio, I. A., Levita, L., Kulkarni, J., Monaco, J., Kentros, C., Stead, M., et al. (2009b). Attention enhances the retrieval and stability of visuospatial and olfactory representations in the dorsal hippocampus. PLoS Biol. 7:e1000140. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000140

Nadel, L., and Hardt, O. (2011). Update on memory systems and processes. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 251–273. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.169

Nikolin, S., Loo, C. K., Bai, S., Dokos, S., and Martin, D. M. (2015). Focalised stimulation using high definition transcranial direct current stimulation (HD-tDCS) to investigate declarative verbal learning and memory functioning. Neuroimage 117, 11–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.019

Norris, D. G. (2000). Reduced power multislice MDEFT imaging. J. Magn. Reson. Imaging 11, 445–451. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1522-2586(200004)11:4&lt;445::AID-JMRI13&gt;3.0.CO;2-T

Nyberg, L. (2002). Levels of processing: a view from functional brain imaging. Memory 10, 345–348. doi: 10.1080/09658210244000171

Nyberg, L., Petersson, K. M., Nilsson, L. G., Sandblom, J., Aberg, C., and Ingvar, M. (2001). Reactivation of motor brain areas during explicit memory for actions. Neuroimage 14, 521–528. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0801

Ofen, N., Kao, Y. C., Sokol-Hessner, P., Kim, H., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., and Gabrieli, J. D. (2007). Development of the declarative memory system in the human brain. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 1198–1205. doi: 10.1038/nn1950

Oldfield, R. C., and Wingfield, A. (1965). Response latencies in naming objects. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 17, 273–281. doi: 10.1080/17470216508416445

Paivio, A. (1969). Mental imagery in associative learning and memory. Psychol. Rev. 76, 241–263. doi: 10.1037/h0027272

Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and Verbal Processes. New York, NY: Holt.

Park, C.-H., Chang, W. H., Lee, M., Kwon, G. H., Kim, L., Kim, S. T., et al. (2015). Which motor cortical region best predicts imagined movement? Neuroimage 113, 101–110. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.033

Pereira, A., Ellis, J., and Freeman, J. (2012). Is prospective memory enhanced by cue-action semantic relatedness and enactment at encoding? Conscious. Cogn. 21 , 1257–1266. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2012.04.012

Pleger, B., and Villringer, A. (2013). The human somatosensory system: from perception to decision making. Progr. Neurobiol. 103, 76–97. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2012.10.002

Pulvermuller, F. (1999). Words in the brain's language. Behav. Brain Sci. 22, 253–279. discussion: 280–336. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X9900182X

Pulvermüller, F. (2001). Brain reflections of words and their meaning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 5, 517–524. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01803-9

Pulvermüller, F. (2002). The Neuroscience of Language: On Brain Circuits of Words and Serial Order . Cambridge; New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Pulvermuller, F. (2005). Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 576–582. doi: 10.1038/nrn1706

Pulvermüller, F. (2013). Semantic embodiment, disembodiment or misembodiment? In search of meaning in modules and neuron circuits. Brain Lang. 127, 86–103. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.05.015

Pulvermüller, F., and Fadiga, L. (2010). Active perception: sensorimotor circuits as a cortical basis for language. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 351–360. doi: 10.1038/nrn2811

Quinn-Allen, L. (1995). The effects of emblematic gestures on the development and access of mental representations of french expressions. Mod. Lang. J. 79, 521–529. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4781.1995.tb05454.x

Ridderinkhof, K. R., and Brass, M. (2015). How kinesthetic motor imagery works: a predictive-processing theory of visualization in sports and motor expertise. J. Physiol. Paris 109, 53–63. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2015.02.003

Rodd, J. M., Johnsrude, I. S., and Davis, M. H. (2012). Dissociating frontotemporal contributions to semantic ambiguity resolution in spoken sentences. Cereb. Cortex 22, 1761–1773. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr252

Saltz, E., and Donnenwerthnolan, S. (1981). Does motoric imagery facilitate memory for sentences - a selective interference test. J. Verbal Learning Verbal Behav. 20, 322–332. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(81)90472-2

Scott, S. K., and Wise, R. J. S. (2004). The functional neuroanatomy of prelexical processing in speech perception. Cognition 92, 13–45. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2002.12.002

Singer, W. (2013). Cortical dynamics revisited. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 616–626. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.006

Smith, C. N., Wixted, J. T., and Squire, L. R. (2011). The hippocampus supports both recollection and familiarity when memories are strong. J. Neurosci. 31, 15693–15702. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3438-11.2011

Soria Bauser, D., and Suchan, B. (2015). Is the whole the sum of its parts? Configural processing of headless bodies in the right fusiform gyrus. Behav. Brain Res. 281, 102–110. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.12.015

Squire, L. R., and Dede, A. J. O. (2015). Conscious and unconscious memory systems. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Biol. 7:a021667. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a021667

St. Jacques, P. L., Kragel, P. A., and Rubin, D. C. (2011). Dynamic neural networks supporting memory retrieval. Neuroimage 57, 608–616. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.04.039

Stoeckel, C., Gough, P. M., Watkins, K. E., and Devlin, J. T. (2009). Supramarginal gyrus involvement in visual word recognition. Cortex 45, 1091–1096. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.12.004

Takashima, A., Bakker, I., Van Hell, J. G., Janzen, G., and McQueen, J. M. (2014). Richness of information about novel words influences how episodic and semantic memory networks interact during lexicalization. Neuroimage 84, 265–278. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.023

Talairach, J., and Tournoux, P. (1988). Co-planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging . Stuttgart: Thieme.

Tellier, M. (2008). The effect of gestures on second language memorisation by young children. Gesture 8, 219–235. doi: 10.1075/gest.8.2.06tel

Tettamanti, M., Buccino, G., Saccuman, M. C., Gallese, V., Danna, M., Scifo, P., et al. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 273–281. doi: 10.1162/0898929053124965

Toumpaniari, K., Loyens, S., Mavilidi, M.-F., and Paas, F. (2015). Preschool children's foreign language vocabulary learning by embodying words through physical activity and gesturing. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 27, 445–456. doi: 10.1007/s10648-015-9316-4

Tulving, E., and Madigan, S. A. (1970). Memory and verbal learning. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 21, 437–484. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.21.020170.002253

Ullman, M. T. (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: the declarative/procedural model. Cognition 92, 231–270. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.10.008

Wilkinson, L., and Jahanshahi, M. (2007). The striatum and probabilistic implicit sequence learning. Brain Res. 1137, 117–130. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.12.051

Wilkinson, L., and Jahanshahi, M. (2015). “Non declarative (Procedural) memory,” in International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 2nd Edn , ed J. D. Wright (Oxford: Elsevier), 844–850.

Wilson, L. B., Tregellas, J. R., Slason, E., Pasko, B. E., and Rojas, D. C. (2011). Implicit phonological priming during visual word recognition. Neuroimage 55, 724–731. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.019

Worsley, K. J., Liao, C. H., Aston, J., Petre, V., Duncan, G. H., Morales, F., et al. (2002). A general statistical analysis for fMRI data. Neuroimage 15, 1–15. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0933

Yamamoto, Y. (2014). Multidimensional vocabulary acquisition through deliberate vocabulary list learning. System 42, 232–243. doi: 10.1016/j.system.2013.12.005

Yin, H. H., and Knowlton, B. J. (2006). The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 464–476. doi: 10.1038/nrn1919

Zaccarella, E., and Friederici, A. D. (2015). Reflections of word processing in the insular cortex: a sub-regional parcellation based functional assessment. Brain Lang. 142, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.12.006

Zhang, J. X., Leung, H.-C., and Johnson, M. K. (2003). Frontal activations associated with accessing and evaluating information in working memory: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 20, 1531–1539. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.07.016

Zhuang, J., Tyler, L. K., Randall, B., Stamatakis, E. A., and Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2014). Optimally efficient neural systems for processing spoken language. Cereb. Cortex 24, 908–918. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs366

Zimmer, H. D. (2001). Memory for Action: A Distinct Form of Episodic Memory? Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Keywords: second language, word learning, enactment, embodiment, brain

Citation: Macedonia M and Mueller K (2016) Exploring the Neural Representation of Novel Words Learned through Enactment in a Word Recognition Task. Front. Psychol. 7:953. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00953

Received: 26 November 2015; Accepted: 09 June 2016; Published: 28 June 2016.

Reviewed by:

Copyright © 2016 Macedonia and Mueller. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY) . The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Karsten Mueller, [email protected]

IMAGES

  1. psychology words image

    psychology task words

  2. Psychology Words

    psychology task words

  3. Introduction to Psychology Vocabulary Task Cards

    psychology task words

  4. psychology words

    psychology task words

  5. What Is Psychologists’ Favorite Word?

    psychology task words

  6. Psychology Vocabulary Quiz

    psychology task words

VIDEO

  1. Psychology verse

  2. Some special word of psychology

  3. Psychology Facts(2)

  4. Tspsc and other psc question task words

  5. Which question is better

  6. Two paragraphs

COMMENTS

  1. Psychology task 1 Essay

    Psychology Task 1 Behaviourists explain behaviour as a response to a stimulus and that of the environment that we live in is the stimulus that we respond

  2. Psychology Tasks Analysis Research Paper Example

    The paper "Psychology Tasks Analysis" focuses on the critical analysis of the major issues on a set of psychology tasks

  3. Psychology task 5000 words

    If you want your psychology task to be done by a qualified and professional academic writer; please feel free to contact me at your earliest

  4. 'simon task' related words: psychology aircraft [2 more]

    Below is a list of words related to simon task. The algorithm isn't perfect, but it does a pretty good job for common-ish words. Here's the list of words that are related to simon task:psychology

  5. Complete 150 Word Psychology Task (ATC)

    YOU WILL COMPLETE THIS TASK TWICE FOR TWO DIFFERENT STUDENTS IN THE SAME CLASS…THEY MUST BE DIFFERENT AND PLEASE.UPLOAD WITH A TURNITIN REPORT PSYC 1101 Assignment – Understanding What Psychologist Do One of the best resources on […]

  6. SOLUTION: Complete 150 Word Psychology Task (ATC)

    Your textbook author discusses 15 areas of study within the field of psychology. This will often pique the interest of the beginning student and often leads to the question, “What career opportunities are there in psychology?

  7. complete 150 word psychology task atc

    complete 150 word psychology task atc. Is this question part of your assignment? We can help ORDER NOW

  8. Edxecel GCSE Psychology 9-1 Social Influence key word task sheet

    This worksheet is given at the beginning of the topic and students complete the descriptions as a starter for each lesson

  9. Frontiers

    Here we explore how enacted words are functionally represented in the brain and which brain regions contribute to enhance retention. After an enactment training lasting 4 days